Loading...
The Rural Voice, 1992-02, Page 24important, and it can be overcome by seeing how hard an animal will work to get or avoid something. In such an experiment, I. J. H. Duncan and V. G. Kite taught chickens to use nesting boxes, and then placed annoying things like air blasts and foot baths in the way to see how determined the birds were to use them. They found that the chickens were "highly motivated" to reach the boxes, a motivation equivalent to the determina- tion they exhibit to reach food after being without it for 20 hours. Marian Stamp Dawkins and Christine Nicol describe a French experiment in which hens were placed in a cage with one moveable wall. The wall moved outwards whcn the hcns pecked at a key, and moved back in when they stopped. The birds pecked at the key at "a rate which kept the wall well away from them, giv- ing each hen at least 625 square centimetres." The example given by Dawkins and Nicol was included in a discussion of the amount of space allo- cated to laying hens by the European Commission, and it illustrates how research into animal welfare is be- ginning to conflict with many of the industry standards. European chickens must now have a minimum of 450 square centimetres of floor space. Using a video camera and a computer analyzer, Dawkins and Sylvie Hardie measured the amount of space the birds occupy when involved in a range of activi- ties, including standing still. Ross Brown hens weighing two kilograms take up 428 to 592 square centimetres standing still. They occupy 978 to 1626 square centimetres turning around, and 1085 to 2060 square cen- timetres flapping their wings. Quot- ing British Ministry of Agriculture, Food, and Fisheries guidelines which say farm animals must be given "free- dom of movement" and "the opportu- nity to exercise most normal patterns of behaviour," Dawkins and Nicol say it "is clear from our study that 450 square centimetres comes no- where near giving hens the opportu- nity to preen, turn around, flap their wings, or even just stand in any way that could be described as `freely'." Dawkins and Nicol found that hens essentially stop trying to move work hard to gain access to soil for rooting, and prefer to spend time in stalls with other pigs beside them. Stereotypic behaviour in tethered sows can be eliminated by ensuring that they have no food restrictions, as the behaviour arises from frustrated foraging instincts in the hungry pig. New types of housing — which meet more of the animal's needs and improve its welfare — are therefore necessary, and much re- search is being conducted into improved designs. However, this is proving to be nearly as difficult as determining which beha- viours are needs in the first place, because not only must the housing improve the animal's wel- fare, but it must also be economically efficient. Improving welfare does not mean throwing out ex- isting technology. The cages now in use do have definite good points, be- cause they were designed to meet very basic welfare standards. A battery cage in a controlled environ- ment is hard to beat when it comes to matters of dis- ease control and clear ac- cess to food. In an article discussing improved "in any fashion" when kept in battery cages. There have been suggestions that, under these conditions, hens for- get how to be active, but Dawkins and Nicol say that when a hen is re- leased from its cage "it begins to stretch and flap its wings at a very high rate; it is still highly motivated to exercise." These types of findings are being published with increasing frequency. It is clear that as well as needing food and water, hens appear to need perch- es, dust baths, and mouldable nesting material in a larger cage in order to be considered healthy. A plaything keeps piglets happy and reduces ag- gressive behaviour, while sows need well -bedded farrowing areas with three to four walls. Pigs will also housing for chickens, Christine Nicol and Marian Stamp Dawkins say that some of the current alternatives to the cages are in fact worse for the chickens. Some free- range systems have a large number of birds housed in a central region. In- explicably, few birds venture out of this crowded place. Nicol and Daw- kins speculate it may be because the birds can't find the exits, or they are prevented from leaving by dominant birds. Or it may be that "the outside has little to offer." Most free-range systems apparently provide grass in- stead of the dense vegetation that chickens prefer, so "all too often the result is a patch of mud near the house, and overgrown, unused grass further away." In order to better meet a chicken's 20 THE RURAL VOICE